Différences
Cette page vous donne les différences entre la révision choisie et la version actuelle de la page.
bibliographie:grady_tower [2025/02/02 16:19] luc [Écrits et publications] |
bibliographie:grady_tower [2025/02/02 16:50] (Version actuelle) luc [Écrits et publications] |
||
---|---|---|---|
Ligne 208: | Ligne 208: | ||
__Distribution of intelligence test scores for five general fields of the doctorate | __Distribution of intelligence test scores for five general fields of the doctorate | ||
- | and for the total doctorate population__ Â | + | and for the total doctorate population:__Â |
- | Army Standard Approx. gen Doctorates (N) | + | Â |
+ | Army Standard Approx. gen __________________Doctorates (N)___________________ | ||
Scale dist population All Phys Biol Social Arts, Educ- | Scale dist population All Phys Biol Social Arts, Educ- | ||
age 32, 1958 fields sciences sciences sciences human. cation | age 32, 1958 fields sciences sciences sciences human. cation | ||
Ligne 224: | Ligne 225: | ||
70-79 218,200 7 1 5 1 | 70-79 218,200 7 1 5 1 | ||
Below 70 162,600 | Below 70 162,600 | ||
+ | –––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––– | ||
Total 2,400,000 3567 1233 645 827 437 425 | Total 2,400,000 3567 1233 645 827 437 425 | ||
100 (mean) 130.8 134.7 126.1 132.3 132.1 123.3 | 100 (mean) 130.8 134.7 126.1 132.3 132.1 123.3 | ||
Ligne 230: | Ligne 231: | ||
- | Terman's statement was based on data provided by the Stanford-Binet. As the remainder of the IQs in this paper will be based on the Wechsler-Bellevue (W-B), or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), it's important to know that Stanford-Binet IQs are typically found to be 10 to 12 points higher than Wechsler-Bellevue scores. In fact, "...the difference between the Revised Stanford-Binet and the Wechsler-Bellevue IQs become progressively greater the higher the IQs are above 100." (Jour of psych XIV, 1942, 317-326. See also Jour of Consul Psych 25, 10 61, 390, and the Jour of Social Psych XXIII, 1946, 237-239) This should not be construed to mean that the Stanford-Binet is less valid than the Wechsler-Bellevue, the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, or any other highly reliable test. In fact, the Stanford-Binet may be somewhat more valid at the very highest levels. All that it does mean is that the level of performance represented by a score on the W-B, or the WAIS, will be represented by a score 10 or 12 points higher on the Stanford-Binet. At the very highest levels this difference will be even greater. | + | Terman's statement was based on data provided by the Stanford-Binet. As the remainder of the IQs in this paper will be based on the Wechsler-Bellevue (W-B), or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), it's important to know that Stanford-Binet IQs are typically found to be 10 to 12 points higher than Wechsler-Bellevue scores. In fact, "...the difference between the Revised Stanford-Binet and the Wechsler-Bellevue IQs become progressively greater the higher the IQs are above 100." (//Jour of psych// XIV, 1942, 317-326. See also //Jour of Consul Psych// 25, 10 61, 390, and the //Jour of Social Psych// XXIII, 1946, 237-239) This should not be construed to mean that the Stanford-Binet is less valid than the Wechsler-Bellevue, the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, or any other highly reliable test. In fact, the Stanford-Binet may be somewhat more valid at the very highest levels. All that it does mean is that the level of performance represented by a score on the W-B, or the WAIS, will be represented by a score 10 or 12 points higher on the Stanford-Binet. At the very highest levels this difference will be even greater. |
- | One of the most outstanding groups of men that I was able to find was that of the faculty of the University of Cambridge. (Nature, 1967, 213, 442) These scores represent the Full Scale WAIS IQs of 148 faculty members in a variety of disciplines from one of the most distinguished Universities in the world. | + | One of the most outstanding groups of men that I was able to find was that of the faculty of the University of Cambridge. (//Nature//, 1967, 213, 442) These scores represent the Full Scale WAIS IQs of 148 faculty members in a variety of disciplines from one of the most distinguished Universities in the world. |
- | The WAIS has a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. Entrance requirements to TNS on the WAIS is a score of 148 (Vidya #7, Nov 1979) | + | The WAIS has a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. Entrance requirements to TNS on the WAIS is a score of 148 (//Vidya// #7, Nov 1979) |
- | Mean, range, variance, and standard error of the mean, for 148 Cambridge faculty  | + | __Mean, range, variance, and standard error of the mean, for 148 Cambridge faculty__ |
- | Mean Range of Standard | + |  |
- | Subject of deviation scores Variance deviation | + | Mean Range of Standard |
- | bachelor degree n IQs of the mean  | + | Subject of deviation scores Variance deviation |
- | Agricultural sciences 17 121.6 110-135 41.18 1.55 | + | bachelor degree n IQs of the mean |
- | Biochemistry 10 130.0 122-141 41.33 2.034Â | + | ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------- Â |
- | Biological sciences 20 126.1 113-135 27.05 1.15Â | + | Agricultural sciences 17 121.6 110-135 41.18 1.55Â |
- | Chemistry 12 129.6 121-138 30.45 1.59Â | + | Biochemistry 10 130.0 122-141 41.33 2.034Â |
- | Engineering sciences 16 125.0 111-138 57.46 1.9Â | + | Biological sciences 20 126.1 113-135 27.05 1.15Â |
- | Mathematics 16 130.4 124-136 16.0 1.0Â | + | Chemistry 12 129.6 121-138 30.45 1.59Â |
- | Medical sciences 10 127.0 116-134 46.22 2.15Â | + | Engineering sciences 16 125.0 111-138 57.46 1.9Â |
- | Physics 20 127.7 112-136 39.05 1.4Â | + | Mathematics 16 130.4 124-136 16.0 1.0Â |
- | Social sciences 10 121.8 112-132 41.55 2.04 | + | Medical sciences 10 127.0 116-134 46.22 2.15Â |
+ | Physics 20 127.7 112-136 39.05 1.4Â | ||
+ | Social sciences 10 121.8 112-132 41.55 2.04 | ||
Ligne 255: | Ligne 258: | ||
- | The following chart is taken from Wechsler's Measurement and Appraisal of | + | The following chart is taken from //Wechsler's Measurement and Appraisal of |
- | Adult Intelligence by Joseph D. Matarazzo, 5th edition, p 177. | + | Adult Intelligence// by Joseph D. Matarazzo, 5th edition, p 177. |
XX | XX | ||
Ligne 268: | Ligne 271: | ||
100 110 120 130 140 150 | 100 110 120 130 140 150 | ||
- | The Full Scale WAIS IQs for 80 medical students. Adapted from Kole, D. M. | + | The Full Scale WAIS IQs for 80 medical students. Adapted from Kole, D. M. |
A study of intellectual and personality characteristics of medical students. | A study of intellectual and personality characteristics of medical students. | ||
- | (The Journal of Medical Education, 1965, 40, 1130-1143) | + | (//The Journal of Medical Education//, 1965, 40, 1130-1143) |
Ligne 277: | Ligne 280: | ||
Matarazzo goes on to say, "Another point which is well understood among the few currently growing group of specialists in this country who deal primarily with the assessment of individuals applying for entry into medicine, law, graduate nursing, physics, economics, psychology, English literature, graduate business school, and a host of related scientific and humanistic disciplines is that, on the basis of a purely intellectual index, such applicants are so similar as to have come from the same population. | Matarazzo goes on to say, "Another point which is well understood among the few currently growing group of specialists in this country who deal primarily with the assessment of individuals applying for entry into medicine, law, graduate nursing, physics, economics, psychology, English literature, graduate business school, and a host of related scientific and humanistic disciplines is that, on the basis of a purely intellectual index, such applicants are so similar as to have come from the same population. | ||
- | Currently practicing physicians are also similar to these young medical students in Full Scale WAIS IQs. (Matarazzo and Goldstein, Journal of Medical Education, 1972, 47, 102-111) | + | Currently practicing physicians are also similar to these young medical students in Full Scale WAIS IQs. (Matarazzo and Goldstein, //Journal of Medical Education//, 1972, 47, 102-111) |
- | Holt and Luborsky in Personality Patterns of Psychiatrists, 1958, p. 40, reported that the "Average Wechsler-Bellevue Scale of Adult Intelligence was 128" ... "Scores ranged from 110 to 145. IQ was not decisive in gaining admittance; the average of those accepted was only slightly higher than those who were rejected. The average verbal subscore was 131 with a range of 115-145." | + | Holt and Luborsky in //Personality Patterns of Psychiatrists//, 1958, p. 40, reported that the "Average Wechsler-Bellevue Scale of Adult Intelligence was 128" ... "Scores ranged from 110 to 145. IQ was not decisive in gaining admittance; the average of those accepted was only slightly higher than those who were rejected. The average verbal subscore was 131 with a range of 115-145." |
- | Balinsky and Shaw (Personnel Psychology, 1956, 9, 207-209) reported on an appraisal of top level executives a Full Scale IQ of 124.09, S.D. 7.90; Verbal Scale IQ 125.1, S.D. 8.09; and Performance IQ 117.09, S.D. 9.81. They also reported that only the Verbal IQ was significantly correlated with performance ratings (r = .32, p<.05). However, the subscale of arithmetical reasoning proved to be the best predictor of management performance (r = .42, p<.01). | + | Balinsky and Shaw (//Personnel Psychology//, 1956, 9, 207-209) reported on an appraisal of top level executives a Full Scale IQ of 124.09, S.D. 7.90; Verbal Scale IQ 125.1, S.D. 8.09; and Performance IQ 117.09, S.D. 9.81. They also reported that only the Verbal IQ was significantly correlated with performance ratings (r = .32, p<.05). However, the subscale of arithmetical reasoning proved to be the best predictor of management performance (r = .42, p<.01). |
- | Up to this point I have tried to avoid using data from less demanding vocations. But because the article by L.M. Simon and E.A. Levitt (Occupations, 1950, 29, 23-25) is the most extensive set of norms I've ever seen in one place for the W-B, I've decided to include their full table. | + | Up to this point I have tried to avoid using data from less demanding vocations. But because the article by L.M. Simon and E.A. Levitt (//Occupations//, 1950, 29, 23-25) is the most extensive set of norms I've ever seen in one place for the W-B, I've decided to include their full table. |
- | SCORES ON THE WECHSLER BELLEVUE SCALE IN RELATION TO OCCUPATION Â | + | SCORES ON THE WECHSLER BELLEVUE SCALE IN RELATION TO OCCUPATION Â |
- | Group N Range 10% Q-1 Median Q-3 90% Â | + | Group N Range 10% Q-1 Median Q-3 90% Â |
- | Engineers 52Â | + | Engineers 52Â |
- | Full 116-148 121 127 133 135 140Â | + | Full 116-148 121 127 133 135 140Â |
- | Verbal 110-144 120 125 130 136 140Â | + | Verbal 110-144 120 125 130 136 140Â |
- | Performance 114-145 119 124 129 133 137Â | + | Performance 114-145 119 124 129 133 137Â |
- | Professionals I 52Â | + | Professionals I 52Â |
- | Full 113-141 120 126 132 134 137Â | + | Full 113-141 120 126 132 134 137Â |
- | Verbal 109-144 121 126 134 136 141Â | + | Verbal 109-144 121 126 134 136 141Â |
- | Performance 99-137 115 119 124 129 132Â | + | Performance 99-137 115 119 124 129 132Â |
- | Educators 45Â | + | Educators 45Â |
- | Full 104-141 118 123 129 134 137Â | + | Full 104-141 118 123 129 134 137Â |
- | Verbal 106-143 112 123 129 134 137Â | + | Verbal 106-143 112 123 129 134 137Â |
- | Performance 95-139 112 117 125 132 135Â | + | Performance 95-139 112 117 125 132 135Â |
- | Professionals II 61Â | + | Professionals II 61Â |
- | Full 106-143 117 123 128 133 138Â | + | Full 106-143 117 123 128 133 138Â |
- | Verbal 112-143 117 123 128 132 137Â | + | Verbal 112-143 117 123 128 132 137Â |
- | Performance 94-141 111 118 124 129 136Â | + | Performance 94-141 111 118 124 129 136Â |
- | Teachers 421Â | + | Teachers 421Â |
- | Full 94-152 114 120 126 132 137Â | + | Full 94-152 114 120 126 132 137Â |
- | Verbal 94-145 113 120 126 131 137Â | + | Verbal 94-145 113 120 126 131 137Â |
- | Performance 83-161 108 115 123 130 134Â | + | Performance 83-161 108 115 123 130 134Â |
- | Social Service 66Â | + | Social Service 66Â |
- | Full 108-145 117 121 125 132 135Â | + | Full 108-145 117 121 125 132 135Â |
- | Verbal 105-145 114 118 124 133 136Â | + | Verbal 105-145 114 118 124 133 136Â |
- | Performance 95-146 106 113 122 129 134Â | + | Performance 95-146 106 113 122 129 134Â |
- | Managers 134Â | + | Managers 134Â |
- | Full 92-146 113 120 125 130 136Â | + | Full 92-146 113 120 125 130 136Â |
- | Verbal 81-140 110 118 124 131 135Â | + | Verbal 81-140 110 118 124 131 135Â |
- | Performance 91-154 109 115 122 129 135Â | + | Performance 91-154 109 115 122 129 135Â |
- | Nurses 191Â | + | Nurses 191Â |
- | Full 93-141 113 118 124 128 132Â | + | Full 93-141 113 118 124 128 132Â |
- | Verbal 99-143 110 115 121 127 131Â | + | Verbal 99-143 110 115 121 127 131Â |
- | Performance 78-145 102 114 125 130 136Â | + | Performance 78-145 102 114 125 130 136Â |
- | Arts 62Â | + | Arts 62Â |
- | Full 74-147 109 117 124 131 138Â | + | Full 74-147 109 117 124 131 138Â |
- | Verbal 73-142 107 115 120 127 136Â | + | Verbal 73-142 107 115 120 127 136Â |
- | Performance 78-145 102 114 125 130 136Â | + | Performance 78-145 102 114 125 130 136Â |
- | Sales 153Â | + | Sales 153Â |
- | Full 95-142 105 112 122 128 132Â | + | Full 95-142 105 112 122 128 132Â |
- | Verbal 93-143 101 111 120 128 133Â | + | Verbal 93-143 101 111 120 128 133Â |
- | Performance 93-144 103 111 118 125 130Â | + | Performance 93-144 103 111 118 125 130Â |
- | Secretaries 107Â | + | Secretaries 107Â |
- | Full 92-135 108 114 121 125 130Â | + | Full 92-135 108 114 121 125 130Â |
- | Verbal 94-134 107 114 120 125 130Â | + | Verbal 94-134 107 114 120 125 130Â |
- | Performance 88-136 102 108 118 126 129Â | + | Performance 88-136 102 108 118 126 129Â |
- | Bookkeepers 55Â | + | Bookkeepers 55Â |
- | Full 99-137 105 109 117 125 129Â | + | Full 99-137 105 109 117 125 129Â |
- | Verbal 98-141 102 107 115 125 129Â | + | Verbal 98-141 102 107 115 125 129Â |
- | Performance 94-141 101 107 114 120 125Â | + | Performance 94-141 101 107 114 120 125Â |
- | Clerks 128Â | + | Clerks 128Â |
- | Full 74-140 97 105 116 122 131Â | + | Full 74-140 97 105 116 122 131Â |
- | Verbal 76-145 99 106 117 122 127Â | + | Verbal 76-145 99 106 117 122 127Â |
- | Performance 69-140 94 103 114 121 129Â | + | Performance 69-140 94 103 114 121 129Â |
- | Office Workers 62Â | + | Office Workers 62Â |
- | Full 88-139 99 108 116 123 128Â | + | Full 88-139 99 108 116 123 128Â |
- | Verbal 89-135 98 104 113 122 127Â | + | Verbal 89-135 98 104 113 122 127Â |
- | Performance 81-141 97 106 115 124 128Â | + | Performance 81-141 97 106 115 124 128Â |
- | Skilled Labor 107Â | + | Skilled Labor 107Â |
- | Full 87-139 103 110 115 123 127Â | + | Full 87-139 103 110 115 123 127Â |
- | Verbal 82-136 98 106 114 120 127Â | + | Verbal 82-136 98 106 114 120 127Â |
- | Performance 93-136 102 108 117 122 126Â | + | Performance 93-136 102 108 117 122 126Â |
- | Personal Service 57Â | + | Personal Service 57Â |
- | Full 54-130 79 92 106 113 122Â | + | Full 54-130 79 92 106 113 122Â |
- | Verbal 59-132 80 90 104 115 121Â | + | Verbal 59-132 80 90 104 115 121Â |
- | Performance 54-129 84 99 105 114 119 | + | Performance 54-129 84 99 105 114 119 |
- | Professionals I -- physicians, dentists, lawyers | + | Professionals I -- physicians, dentists, lawyers |
- | Educators -- college deans and instructors, high school and grammar school principals | + | Educators -- college deans and instructors, high school and grammar school principals |
- | Professionals II -- pharmacists, accountants | + | Professionals II -- pharmacists, accountants |
- | Social Service -- social workers, clergy | + | Social Service -- social workers, clergy |
- | Managers -- small business owners, retail store managers, office managers, foremen | + | Managers -- small business owners, retail store managers, office managers, foremen |
business executives, small manufacturers. | business executives, small manufacturers. | ||
- | Arts -- singers, dancers, musicians, actors, artists, designers, commercial artists | + | Arts -- singers, dancers, musicians, actors, artists, designers, commercial artists |
- | Sales -- all inside and outside sales people, wholesale and retail | + | Sales -- all inside and outside sales people, wholesale and retail |
- | Office Workers -- stenographers, comptometer operators, typists, receptionists, | + | Office Workers -- stenographers, comptometer operators, typists, receptionists, |
telephone switchboard operators | telephone switchboard operators | ||
- | Skilled Labor -- machinists, automobile and aircraft mechanics, radio and television | + | Skilled Labor -- machinists, automobile and aircraft mechanics, radio and television |
repairmen, plumbers, electricians | repairmen, plumbers, electricians | ||
- | Personal Service -- barbers, beauticians, waiters, food handlers, soda fountain | + | Personal Service -- barbers, beauticians, waiters, food handlers, soda fountain |
attendants, domestics | attendants, domestics | ||
Ligne 370: | Ligne 373: | ||
I included this particular reference because there were enough high level professions included in the table to make it of interest to my topic. Readers who are interested in similar scores for a very much larger range of occupations are referred to the Army General Classification Test scores from World War II. most of that data is not based on intellectually demanding occupations and is not included in this paper for that reason. | I included this particular reference because there were enough high level professions included in the table to make it of interest to my topic. Readers who are interested in similar scores for a very much larger range of occupations are referred to the Army General Classification Test scores from World War II. most of that data is not based on intellectually demanding occupations and is not included in this paper for that reason. | ||
- | Just after World War II, Dr. G.M. Gilbert, a psychologist-psychiatrist, was given access to the prisoners who were to be put on trial for war crimes at Nuremberg. Included in his examination was a German version of the American Wechsler-Bellevue Adult Intelligence Test. The results of those tests, presented in the table below, may be found in Dr. Gilbert's book, Nuremberg Diary, and also in The Reich Manual, p. 363, by Leonard Mosley. | + | Just after World War II, Dr. G.M. Gilbert, a psychologist-psychiatrist, was given access to the prisoners who were to be put on trial for war crimes at Nuremberg. Included in his examination was a German version of the American Wechsler-Bellevue Adult Intelligence Test. The results of those tests, presented in the table below, may be found in Dr. Gilbert's book, //Nuremberg Diary//, and also in //The Reich Manual//, p. 363, by Leonard Mosley. |
Name IQ | Name IQ | ||
Ligne 394: | Ligne 397: | ||
20. Ernst Kaltenbrunner 113 | 20. Ernst Kaltenbrunner 113 | ||
21. Julius Streicher 106 | 21. Julius Streicher 106 | ||
+ | |||
It's interesting to speculate on what Adolf Hitler's IQ might have been. I think we're on pretty safe ground in saying that he must have been at least as bright as the top members on this list. If he hadn't been, these wolves would surely have torn him apart. | It's interesting to speculate on what Adolf Hitler's IQ might have been. I think we're on pretty safe ground in saying that he must have been at least as bright as the top members on this list. If he hadn't been, these wolves would surely have torn him apart. | ||
- | TNS accepts a WAIS score of 148 for admission, but does not accept Wechsler-Bellevue scores (Vidya #7, Nov 1979). However, if TNS did accept such scores, the admission requirement would be about 143. The W-B has a standard deviation of about 14 points. This places the median score (129) of the Nazi leaders just above the 98th percentile, and the highest scoring of the group just below TNS admission requirements. | + | TNS accepts a WAIS score of 148 for admission, but does not accept Wechsler-Bellevue scores (//Vidya// #7, Nov 1979). However, if TNS did accept such scores, the admission requirement would be about 143. The W-B has a standard deviation of about 14 points. This places the median score (129) of the Nazi leaders just above the 98th percentile, and the highest scoring of the group just below TNS admission requirements. |
- | It's also interesting to not that Dr. Gilbert was one of Terman's research subjects. He can be easily identified from the clues offered on page 365 of The Gifted Child Grows Up (5th edition, 1976). | + | It's also interesting to not that Dr. Gilbert was one of Terman's research subjects. He can be easily identified from the clues offered on page 365 of //The Gifted Child Grows Up// (5th edition, 1976). |
The evidence presented so far supports two conclusions. First, that practical success in even the most intellectually demanding professions, such as scientist, professor, physician, or high level executive requires an intelligence quotient no higher than the 96th percentile (125 WAIS). Second, that even in these demanding professions, there is a broad range of ability. Some doctorates were even awarded to individuals with IQs below 100, although this was rare. | The evidence presented so far supports two conclusions. First, that practical success in even the most intellectually demanding professions, such as scientist, professor, physician, or high level executive requires an intelligence quotient no higher than the 96th percentile (125 WAIS). Second, that even in these demanding professions, there is a broad range of ability. Some doctorates were even awarded to individuals with IQs below 100, although this was rare. | ||
Ligne 428: | Ligne 432: | ||
In short, after job training or formal education, IQs become relatively ineffective predictors of success. | In short, after job training or formal education, IQs become relatively ineffective predictors of success. | ||
- | For the reader who is interested in the subject of predicting job performance by means of IQ and other kinds of tests, I highly recommend E.E. Ghiselli's book, The Validity of Occupational Aptitude Tests, New York; Wiley, 1966. | + | For the reader who is interested in the subject of predicting job performance by means of IQ and other kinds of tests, I highly recommend E.E. Ghiselli's book, //The Validity of Occupational Aptitude Tests//, New York; Wiley, 1966. |
- | The strongest, clearest, and most comprehensive summary of all these research finding can probably be found in A.R. Jensen's book, Bias in Mental Testing, 1980, p. 113. | + | The strongest, clearest, and most comprehensive summary of all these research finding can probably be found in A.R. Jensen's book, //Bias in Mental Testing//, 1980, p. 113. |
- | "Although IQs are an interval scale, the practical, social, economic and career implications of different IQs most certainly do not represent equal intervals. Again, this is not a fault of the IQ scale, but is the result of personal and societal values and demands. The implications and consequences of, say, a 30-point IQ difference is more significant between IQs of 70 and 100 than between IQs of 130 and 160. The importance of a given difference depends not only on its magnitude, but on whether or not it crosses over any of the social, educational, and occupational thresholds of IQ. To be sure, these thresholds are statistical and represent only differing probabilities for individuals' falling on either side of the threshold. But the differential probabilities are not negligible. Such probabilistic thresholds of this type occur in different regions of the IQ scale, not by arbitrary convention or definition, but because of the structure of the educational and occupational systems of modern industrial societies and their correlated demands on the kind of cognitive ability measured by IQ tests. | + | >"Although IQs are an interval scale, the practical, social, economic and career implications of different IQs most certainly do not represent equal intervals. Again, this is not a fault of the IQ scale, but is the result of personal and societal values and demands. The implications and consequences of, say, a 30-point IQ difference is more significant between IQs of 70 and 100 than between IQs of 130 and 160. The importance of a given difference depends not only on its magnitude, but on whether or not it crosses over any of the social, educational, and occupational thresholds of IQ. To be sure, these thresholds are statistical and represent only differing probabilities for individuals' falling on either side of the threshold. But the differential probabilities are not negligible. Such probabilistic thresholds of this type occur in different regions of the IQ scale, not by arbitrary convention or definition, but because of the structure of the educational and occupational systems of modern industrial societies and their correlated demands on the kind of cognitive ability measured by IQ tests. |
- | The four socially and personally most important most important threshold regions on the IQ scale are those that differentiate with high probability between persons who, because of their level of general mental ability, can or cannot attend a regular school (about IQ 50), can or cannot master the traditional subject matter of elementary school (about IQ 75), can or cannot succeed in the academic or college preparatory curriculum through high school (about IQ 105), can or cannot graduate from an accredited four-year college with grades that would qualify for admission to a professional or graduate school (about IQ 115). beyond this, the IQ level becomes relatively unimportant in terms of ordinary occupational aspirations and criteria of success. That is not to say that there are not real differences between the intellectual capabilities represented by IQs of 115 and 150 or even between IQs of 150 and 180. But IQ differences in this upper part of the scale have far less personal implications than the thresholds just described and are generally of lesser importance for success in the popular sense than are certain traits of personality and character. | + | >The four socially and personally most important most important threshold regions on the IQ scale are those that differentiate with high probability between persons who, because of their level of general mental ability, can or cannot attend a regular school (about IQ 50), can or cannot master the traditional subject matter of elementary school (about IQ 75), can or cannot succeed in the academic or college preparatory curriculum through high school (about IQ 105), can or cannot graduate from an accredited four-year college with grades that would qualify for admission to a professional or graduate school (about IQ 115). beyond this, the IQ level becomes relatively unimportant in terms of ordinary occupational aspirations and criteria of success. That is not to say that there are not real differences between the intellectual capabilities represented by IQs of 115 and 150 or even between IQs of 150 and 180. But IQ differences in this upper part of the scale have far less personal implications than the thresholds just described and are generally of lesser importance for success in the popular sense than are certain traits of personality and character. |
- | The social implications of exceptionally high ability and its interaction with the other factors that make for unusual achievements are considerably greater than the personal implications. The quality of a society's culture is highly determined by the very small fraction of its population that is most exceptionally endowed. The growth of civilization, the development of written language and of mathematics, the great religious and philosophic insights, scientific discoveries, practical inventions, industrial developments, advancements in legal and political systems, and the world's masterpieces of literature, architecture, music and painting, it seems safe to say, are attributable to a rare small proportion of the human population throughout history who undoubtedly possessed, in addition to other important qualities of talent, energy, and imagination, a high level of the essential mental ability measured by tests of intelligence." | + | >The social implications of exceptionally high ability and its interaction with the other factors that make for unusual achievements are considerably greater than the personal implications. The quality of a society's culture is highly determined by the very small fraction of its population that is most exceptionally endowed. The growth of civilization, the development of written language and of mathematics, the great religious and philosophic insights, scientific discoveries, practical inventions, industrial developments, advancements in legal and political systems, and the world's masterpieces of literature, architecture, music and painting, it seems safe to say, are attributable to a rare small proportion of the human population throughout history who undoubtedly possessed, in addition to other important qualities of talent, energy, and imagination, a high level of the essential mental ability measured by tests of intelligence." |
Terman was right; an IQ above 140 has very little significance in terms of personal success. But if Jensen's assessment is correct, and I am confident that it is, where were the historical personages that should have emerged from Terman's study? | Terman was right; an IQ above 140 has very little significance in terms of personal success. But if Jensen's assessment is correct, and I am confident that it is, where were the historical personages that should have emerged from Terman's study? | ||
- | Terman suffered badly from halo effect He not only exaggerated the importance of IQ, he also inflated test scores. All of his subjects were chosen as children of approximately 11 years of age. Their childhood IQs were reported to be 152, but Quinn McNemar reported their adult IQs to be only 134, 18 points less than their childhood scores (The Gifted Child Grows Up, p. 146). Yet adult IQs were avoided as often as possible in reporting adult achievements; childhood IQs were preferred in every case. Other investigators making use of Terman's data almost always follow this practice as well. Consequently, a myth has grown up reflecting a superiority that Terman's group did not in fact possess. | + | Terman suffered badly from halo effect He not only exaggerated the importance of IQ, he also inflated test scores. All of his subjects were chosen as children of approximately 11 years of age. Their childhood IQs were reported to be 152, but Quinn McNemar reported their adult IQs to be only 134, 18 points less than their childhood scores (//The Gifted Child Grows Up//, p. 146). Yet adult IQs were avoided as often as possible in reporting adult achievements; childhood IQs were preferred in every case. Other investigators making use of Terman's data almost always follow this practice as well. Consequently, a myth has grown up reflecting a superiority that Terman's group did not in fact possess. |
- | Terman also selected members of his group with IQs above 170 (average 177.7 men and 177.6 women) for special study. If we subtract the same 18 point difference from 170 that we found between childhood IQs and adult IQs in the main group, we will arrive at an estimate of 152: an estimate less than the TNS average of 156. But even 152 is an overestimate for two reasons. First, the farther a score is from the mean, the greater the regression to the mean will be: 18 points is an underestimate. Second, Terman says specifically that an IQ of 170 is found about 3 times in 10,000 of the general population (The Gifted Child Grows Up, p. 282). That's 3.43 sigma according to my tables, or about 155 IQ, before the 18 or more points are subtracted to allow for adult regression. The average TNS member is at least the equal of Terman's high group, and may very well be their superior. | + | Terman also selected members of his group with IQs above 170 (average 177.7 men and 177.6 women) for special study. If we subtract the same 18 point difference from 170 that we found between childhood IQs and adult IQs in the main group, we will arrive at an estimate of 152: an estimate less than the TNS average of 156. But even 152 is an overestimate for two reasons. First, the farther a score is from the mean, the greater the regression to the mean will be: 18 points is an underestimate. Second, Terman says specifically that an IQ of 170 is found about 3 times in 10,000 of the general population (//The Gifted Child Grows Up//, p. 282). That's 3.43 sigma according to my tables, or about 155 IQ, before the 18 or more points are subtracted to allow for adult regression. The average TNS member is at least the equal of Terman's high group, and may very well be their superior. |
Nevertheless, Terman's "Subjects of IQ 170 or Above" is the only group that I'm aware of that comes close to furnishing a picture of what TNS members are probably like. | Nevertheless, Terman's "Subjects of IQ 170 or Above" is the only group that I'm aware of that comes close to furnishing a picture of what TNS members are probably like. | ||
- | Jensen's reference to the social implications of exceptionally high IQs suggests that intellectual achievements on an historical scale will be made only by those of very rare ability indeed. Can we find such achievements and measure the IQs of those who made them? Indeed we can. In fact, it's already been done. In the early 1950s, Doctor Anne Roe wrote a book titled The Making of a Scientist (Greenwood Press, Westport Conn, 1973) in which she investigated the biographies, personalities, and intellectual abilities of 64 of America's most eminent living scientists, some of whom were Nobel Prize winners. She found the average verbal IQ of this very eminent group to be 166 (p. 164). These scientists comprise the only research group I've come across whose intellectual abilities resemble those of the Four Sigma Society. | + | Jensen's reference to the social implications of exceptionally high IQs suggests that intellectual achievements on an historical scale will be made only by those of very rare ability indeed. Can we find such achievements and measure the IQs of those who made them? Indeed we can. In fact, it's already been done. In the early 1950s, Doctor Anne Roe wrote a book titled //The Making of a Scientist// (Greenwood Press, Westport Conn, 1973) in which she investigated the biographies, personalities, and intellectual abilities of 64 of America's most eminent living scientists, some of whom were Nobel Prize winners. She found the average verbal IQ of this very eminent group to be 166 (p. 164). These scientists comprise the only research group I've come across whose intellectual abilities resemble those of the Four Sigma Society. |
CODA | CODA | ||
- | If IQs above 140 have little importance for personal achievement, what then is left for us? Jensen says, "The evidence is overwhelming that scholastic achievement increases linearly as a function of IQ throughout the entire range of the IQ scale..." (Bias in Mental Testing, p. 319). In other words, what's left for us is knowledge. We are the ones who learn, who understand, who discover, and who invent. That should be ample challenge for any of us. </spoiler> | + | If IQs above 140 have little importance for personal achievement, what then is left for us? Jensen says, "The evidence is overwhelming that scholastic achievement increases linearly as a function of IQ throughout the entire range of the IQ scale..." (//Bias in Mental Testing//, p. 319). In other words, what's left for us is knowledge. We are the ones who learn, who understand, who discover, and who invent. That should be ample challenge for any of us. </spoiler> |
* [[http://www.freerepublic.com/focus/f-chat/2242985/posts|Theories of Multiple Intelligence]] | * [[http://www.freerepublic.com/focus/f-chat/2242985/posts|Theories of Multiple Intelligence]] | ||