Différences
Cette page vous donne les différences entre la révision choisie et la version actuelle de la page.
bibliographie:grady_tower [2015/09/21 14:57] luc |
bibliographie:grady_tower [2025/02/02 16:50] (Version actuelle) luc [Écrits et publications] |
||
---|---|---|---|
Ligne 3: | Ligne 3: | ||
===== Biographie ===== | ===== Biographie ===== | ||
- | Grady M Towers était un [[articles:THQI|THQI]] faisant partie de l'organisation [[wp>Triple_Nine_Society|Triple Nine Society]] ([[http://www.polymath-systems.com/intel/hiqsocs/hiqarch/tnregent/tnsexa96.html|confirmé]]), de [[wp>Prometheus_Society|The Prometheus Society]] ([[http://www.prometheussociety.org/officers/index.html|confirmé]]), de [[wp>Mega_Society|The Mega Society]] ([[http://www.megasociety.net/noesis/149/|confirmé ?]]), de [[http://www.toponesociety.com|Top One Percent Society]] (à confirmer). | + | Grady M Towers était un [[articles:THQI|THQI]] faisant partie de l'organisation [[wp>Triple_Nine_Society|Triple Nine Society]] ([[http://www.polymath-systems.com/intel/hiqsocs/hiqarch/tnregent/tnsexa96.html|confirmé]]), de [[wp>Prometheus_Society|The Prometheus Society]] ([[http://www.prometheussociety.org/officers/index.html|confirmé]]), de [[wp>Mega_Society|The Mega Society]] ([[http://www.megasociety.net/noesis/149/|confirmé]]), de [[http://www.toponesociety.com|Top One Percent Society]] (à confirmer). |
- | A une époque, il a été anthropologue, et a vécu plusieurs années avec une tribu indienne. | + | À une époque, il a été anthropologue, et a vécu plusieurs années avec une tribu indienne. |
Il a été en contact avec les sociétés pour très hauts QI pendant des années, principalement en écrivant des articles et en les proposant à la publication dans le journal de ces sociétés, et en maintenant une correspondance avec quelques personnes qu'il a sélectionnées avec soin. Il a contribué à l'étalonnage de tests utilisés par The Mega Society (une personne sur un million). | Il a été en contact avec les sociétés pour très hauts QI pendant des années, principalement en écrivant des articles et en les proposant à la publication dans le journal de ces sociétés, et en maintenant une correspondance avec quelques personnes qu'il a sélectionnées avec soin. Il a contribué à l'étalonnage de tests utilisés par The Mega Society (une personne sur un million). | ||
Ligne 18: | Ligne 18: | ||
* [[http://prometheussociety.org/cms/index.php?option=com_content&view=article&id=1:theoutsiders&catid=10:articles-favorite&Itemid=103|The Outsiders]] -- les recherches de [[wp>Lewis_Terman|Lewis M. Terman]] et de [[wp>Leta_Stetter_Hollingworth|Leta S. Hollingworth]] revisitées : il en ressort qu'un THQI a jusqu'à 3 fois plus de chances de développer des problèmes d'ajustement et des problèmes psychologiques, et qu'il a une forte probabilité de beaucoup s'ennuyer en classe, d'[[articles:scanneur|être un scanneur]] et [[articles:TDAH]], d'avoir [[zcf>t829-le-probleme-consistant-a-avoir-trop-d-aptitudes|« trop d'aptitudes »]], mais pas celle de supporter les idiots, et finalement de se retrouver en [[zcf>t748-le-qi-et-le-probleme-de-l-inadaptation-sociale|isolement social]], avec la sensation d'être un étranger. | * [[http://prometheussociety.org/cms/index.php?option=com_content&view=article&id=1:theoutsiders&catid=10:articles-favorite&Itemid=103|The Outsiders]] -- les recherches de [[wp>Lewis_Terman|Lewis M. Terman]] et de [[wp>Leta_Stetter_Hollingworth|Leta S. Hollingworth]] revisitées : il en ressort qu'un THQI a jusqu'à 3 fois plus de chances de développer des problèmes d'ajustement et des problèmes psychologiques, et qu'il a une forte probabilité de beaucoup s'ennuyer en classe, d'[[articles:scanneur|être un scanneur]] et [[articles:TDAH]], d'avoir [[zcf>t829-le-probleme-consistant-a-avoir-trop-d-aptitudes|« trop d'aptitudes »]], mais pas celle de supporter les idiots, et finalement de se retrouver en [[zcf>t748-le-qi-et-le-probleme-de-l-inadaptation-sociale|isolement social]], avec la sensation d'être un étranger. | ||
- | <spoiler |The Outsiders – A defination extremely high IQ above 170 apply to themselves - texte complet > | + | <spoiler |The Outsiders – A defination extremely high IQ above 170 apply to themselves – texte complet > |
His name was William James Sidis, and his IQ was estimated at between 250 and 300 [8, p. 283]. At eighteen months he could read The New York Times, at two he taught himself Latin, at three he learned Greek. By the time he was an adult he could speak more than forty languages and dialects. He gained entrance to Harvard at eleven, and gave a lecture on four-dimensional bodies to the Harvard Mathematical Club his first year. He graduated cum laude at sixteen, and became the youngest professor in history. He deduced the possibility of black holes more than twenty years before Subrahmanyan Chandrasekhar published An Introduction to the Study of Stellar Structure. His life held possibilities for achievement that few people can imagine. Of all the prodigies for which there are records, his was probably the most powerful intellect of all. And yet it all came to nothing. He soon gave up his position as a professor, and for the rest of his life wandered from one menial job to another. His experiences as a child prodigy had proven so painful that he decided for the rest of his life to shun public exposure at all costs. Henceforth, he denied his gifts, refused to think about mathematics, and above all refused to perform as he had been made to do as a child. Instead, he devoted his intellect almost exclusively to the collection of streetcar transfers, and to the study of the history of his native Boston. He worked hard at becoming a normal human being, but never entirely succeeded. He found the concept of beauty, for example, to be completely incomprehensible, and the idea of sex repelled him. At fifteen he took a vow of celibacy, which he apparently kept for the remainder of his life, dying a virgin at the age of 46. He wore a vest summer and winter, and never learned to bathe regularly. A comment that Aldous Huxley once made about Sir Isaac Newton might equally have been said of Sidis.\\ | His name was William James Sidis, and his IQ was estimated at between 250 and 300 [8, p. 283]. At eighteen months he could read The New York Times, at two he taught himself Latin, at three he learned Greek. By the time he was an adult he could speak more than forty languages and dialects. He gained entrance to Harvard at eleven, and gave a lecture on four-dimensional bodies to the Harvard Mathematical Club his first year. He graduated cum laude at sixteen, and became the youngest professor in history. He deduced the possibility of black holes more than twenty years before Subrahmanyan Chandrasekhar published An Introduction to the Study of Stellar Structure. His life held possibilities for achievement that few people can imagine. Of all the prodigies for which there are records, his was probably the most powerful intellect of all. And yet it all came to nothing. He soon gave up his position as a professor, and for the rest of his life wandered from one menial job to another. His experiences as a child prodigy had proven so painful that he decided for the rest of his life to shun public exposure at all costs. Henceforth, he denied his gifts, refused to think about mathematics, and above all refused to perform as he had been made to do as a child. Instead, he devoted his intellect almost exclusively to the collection of streetcar transfers, and to the study of the history of his native Boston. He worked hard at becoming a normal human being, but never entirely succeeded. He found the concept of beauty, for example, to be completely incomprehensible, and the idea of sex repelled him. At fifteen he took a vow of celibacy, which he apparently kept for the remainder of his life, dying a virgin at the age of 46. He wore a vest summer and winter, and never learned to bathe regularly. A comment that Aldous Huxley once made about Sir Isaac Newton might equally have been said of Sidis.\\ | ||
\\ | \\ | ||
Ligne 162: | Ligne 162: | ||
[[http://hiqnews.megafoundation.org/Terman_Summary.htm|The Terman Study]] : Ce commentaire de l'étude de Terman n'est pas de Grady M. Towers, mais cite The Outsiders | [[http://hiqnews.megafoundation.org/Terman_Summary.htm|The Terman Study]] : Ce commentaire de l'étude de Terman n'est pas de Grady M. Towers, mais cite The Outsiders | ||
- | * [[http://www.eskimo.com/~miyaguch/grady/emptypromise.html|The Empty Promise]] -- Après que [[wp>Lewis_Terman|Lewis M. Terman]] ait indiqué qu'un QI très élevé dans l'enfance n'était pas un prédicteur de réussite sociale, l'auteur tente de démontrer que la réussite sociale nécessite un haut QI, et y parvient partiellement (QI moyen de ceux "qui ont réussi" : 125, avec une forte dispersion). Note : l'auteur n'étudie pas l'hypothèse selon laquelle un très haut QI dans l'enfance pourrait être un handicap à la réussite sociale, pour cause d'isolement social. | + | * [[https://web.archive.org/web/20200123163201/https://miyaguchi.4sigma.org/gradytowers/emptypromise.html|The Empty Promise]] -- Après que [[wp>Lewis_Terman|Lewis M. Terman]] ait indiqué qu'un QI très élevé dans l'enfance n'était pas un prédicteur de réussite sociale, l'auteur tente de démontrer que la réussite sociale nécessite un haut QI, et y parvient partiellement (QI moyen de ceux "qui ont réussi" : 125, avec une forte dispersion). Note : l'auteur n'étudie pas l'hypothèse selon laquelle un très haut QI dans l'enfance pourrait être un handicap à la réussite sociale, pour cause d'isolement social. |
- | * Theories of Multiple Intelligence | + | |
- | <spoiler |Theories of Multiple Intelligence - texte complet >The mathematical technique called factor analysis was invented by psychologists specifically to answer the age old question, "Is there more than one kind of intelligence?" We now know that there are two: one called fluid g, measured by culture fair tests such as the Raven Progressive Matrices or the LAIT, and another called crystallized g, measured by culture loaded tests like the Concept Mastery Test or the Miller Analogies Test. What we call g has been defined as the ability to "educe relations and correlates," or in more everyday terms, the abilities for inductive ("relations") and deductive ("correlates") reasoning. Culture fair tests measure the ability to educe relations and correlates using abstract diagrams, and other material that requires only a minimum of formal learning. Culture loaded tests measure the ability to educe relations and correlates using learned and over learned material, such as vocabulary, algorithms for arithmetic or multiplication, recognition of common objects and their uses, etc. Ordinary IQ tests measure both kinds of intelligence, but not necessarily to the same degree; they are generally biased in favor of crystallized g.\\ | + | <spoiler |The Empty Promise - Texte complet >In 1921, Lewis M. Terman, by means of teacher selection and multiple testing, identified 1,500 of the most intelligent children in the California school system. His aim was to follow this group of gifted people throughout their lives and observe their achievements in the widest possible sense. He was interested not only in their educational and vocational successes, but also their hobbies, interests, reading habits, health, and many other things. |
+ | Â | ||
+ | As children, the Terman group was larger and heavier than the average California child; they matured earlier and their health was better. They averaged one full grade ahead of their contemporaries in grade placement, while at the same time more than half of them had mastered the curriculum a full two or more grades beyond their classmates. There was no evidence that the gifted group was cursed with any compensatory weaknesses, but because they did prefer older playmates and more solitary play, they were sometimes reported by others to be "queer" or "different". Nevertheless, they were not thought to be any more unsociable or unpopular than other children. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | As adults, most went on to college and many earned academic distinctions. Delinquency and crime were both below average in frequency, and suicide rates, marriage rates, and age of marriage were the same, or nearly the same as those of the general population. The group's war record, occupational success, and income was clearly superior. By age 35, they had published 90 books and 1,500 articles. By age 45, the group had produced 2,000 scientific papers, 230 patents, and 33 novels, as well as many shorter literary works. All in all, the general picture was one of high scholastic achievement and pronounced occupational success. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Terman entitled his longitudinal research project //Genetic Studies of Genius//, and quite clearly, this was exactly what Terman expected it to be. But soon the evidence began to mount that IQ was not the index of genius that Terman had first thought. There was no John F. Kennedy, no Robert Oppenheimer, no J.B.S. Haldane, and not even a Henry Ford. Clearly, IQ was the single most important variable ever discovered for the prediction of achievement, but it was just as clear that genius was too elusive to be captured by the IQ net alone. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Finally, in the //39th Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education Part I//, pp. 83-84, Terman made a most astonishing statement. "Our conclusion is that for subjects brought up under present-day educational regimes, excess in IQ above 140 or 150 adds little to one's achievement in the early adult years." A little farther on he says, "The data reviewed indicate that, above the IQ level of 140, adult success is largely determined by such factors as social adjustment, emotional stability, and drive to accomplishment."Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | In other word, an extremely high IQ conveys no practical advantages at all. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | For a man who had devoted most of his life to the study of gifted people, this was a painful admission for him to make. It is patently obvious that he would not have made it unless he sincerely believed it to be true. But was it true? Was Terman right? | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | I decided to find out. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The thing to do, it seemed to me, was to reverse Terman's procedure. Find as many intellectually demanding fields as possible, for which there were documented IQ records, and check them for measured intelligence. In other words, use demonstrable, real-world accomplishment as a check against the efficacy of the IQ as a predictor. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | I began with a summary of IQs found for doctorates awarded in 1958 (//Science//, 1961, Vol 133, Jan-Jun, 679-688). The test used for these comparisons was the Army General Classification Test, which has a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 20. A score of 162 on the AGCT is required for admission to TNS (//Vidya// #7, Nov 1979). | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | I also took the liberty of computing an estimate of LAIT equivalent scores as an aid in making comparisons. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | __Mean and standard deviation of the doctorates for 1958 on the AGCT and the LAIT__Â | ||
+ | AGCT scores LAIT scores | ||
+ | Field of doctorate Mean S.D. Mean S.D. Â | ||
+ | Math 138.2 17.0 130.6 13.6Â | ||
+ | Physics 140.3 16.4 132.2 13.1Â | ||
+ | Chemistry 131.5 16.3 125.2 13.0Â | ||
+ | Geology 133.1 14.7 126.5 11.8Â | ||
+ | Engineering 134.8 16.2 127.8 13.0Â | ||
+ | All physical sciences 134.7 16.6 127.8 13.3Â | ||
+ | Biological sciences 126.1 16.4 120.8 13.1Â | ||
+ | Social sciences 132.0 16.9 125.6 13.5Â | ||
+ | Arts and humanities 132.1 16.4 125.7 13.1Â | ||
+ | Education 123.3 16.2 118.6 13.0Â | ||
+ | Social sciences, arts, educ 129.8 17.0 123.8 13.6Â | ||
+ | Natural sciences 131.7 17.0 125.4 13.6Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Total 130.8 17.1 124.6 13.7 Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The following chart presents the same data in a somewhat different way. Using this format, it's a simple matter to compute percentages falling above or below any given IQ level. For example, more than 70 percent of all doctorates for 1958 had IQs below the 98th percentile. Even in the physical sciences, more than 60 percent fell below the 98th percentile, and only about 5 percent were above TNS admission standards. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | __Distribution of intelligence test scores for five general fields of the doctorate | ||
+ | and for the total doctorate population:__Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Army Standard Approx. gen __________________Doctorates (N)___________________ Â | ||
+ | Scale dist population All Phys Biol Social Arts, Educ-Â | ||
+ | age 32, 1958 fields sciences sciences sciences human. cation  | ||
+ | 170-and up 530 46 20 1 15 7 3Â | ||
+ | 160-169 2,670 101 46 11 27 14 3Â | ||
+ | 150-159 12,150 337 153 37 93 35 19Â | ||
+ | 140-149 39,250 530 246 67 112 74 31Â | ||
+ | 130-139 108,000 826 298 150 179 116 83Â | ||
+ | 120-129 218,200 806 243 153 214 89 107Â | ||
+ | 110-119 361,800 520 140 119 106 65 90Â | ||
+ | 100-109 457,400 298 64 79 67 28 60Â | ||
+ | 90-99 457,400 81 19 19 12 8 23Â | ||
+ | 80-89 361,800 15 3 4 2 6Â | ||
+ | 70-79 218,200 7 1 5 1Â | ||
+ | Below 70 162,600Â | ||
+ | –––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––––– | ||
+ | Total 2,400,000 3567 1233 645 827 437 425Â | ||
+ | 100 (mean) 130.8 134.7 126.1 132.3 132.1 123.3Â | ||
+ | No information 4220 931 676 924 771 918 Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Terman's statement was based on data provided by the Stanford-Binet. As the remainder of the IQs in this paper will be based on the Wechsler-Bellevue (W-B), or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), it's important to know that Stanford-Binet IQs are typically found to be 10 to 12 points higher than Wechsler-Bellevue scores. In fact, "...the difference between the Revised Stanford-Binet and the Wechsler-Bellevue IQs become progressively greater the higher the IQs are above 100." (//Jour of psych// XIV, 1942, 317-326. See also //Jour of Consul Psych// 25, 10 61, 390, and the //Jour of Social Psych// XXIII, 1946, 237-239) This should not be construed to mean that the Stanford-Binet is less valid than the Wechsler-Bellevue, the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, or any other highly reliable test. In fact, the Stanford-Binet may be somewhat more valid at the very highest levels. All that it does mean is that the level of performance represented by a score on the W-B, or the WAIS, will be represented by a score 10 or 12 points higher on the Stanford-Binet. At the very highest levels this difference will be even greater. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | One of the most outstanding groups of men that I was able to find was that of the faculty of the University of Cambridge. (//Nature//, 1967, 213, 442) These scores represent the Full Scale WAIS IQs of 148 faculty members in a variety of disciplines from one of the most distinguished Universities in the world. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The WAIS has a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. Entrance requirements to TNS on the WAIS is a score of 148 (//Vidya// #7, Nov 1979)Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | __Mean, range, variance, and standard error of the mean, for 148 Cambridge faculty__Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Mean Range of Standard | ||
+ | Subject of deviation scores Variance deviation | ||
+ | bachelor degree n IQs of the mean | ||
+ | ---------------------------------------------------------------------------------- Â | ||
+ | Agricultural sciences 17 121.6 110-135 41.18 1.55Â | ||
+ | Biochemistry 10 130.0 122-141 41.33 2.034Â | ||
+ | Biological sciences 20 126.1 113-135 27.05 1.15Â | ||
+ | Chemistry 12 129.6 121-138 30.45 1.59Â | ||
+ | Engineering sciences 16 125.0 111-138 57.46 1.9Â | ||
+ | Mathematics 16 130.4 124-136 16.0 1.0Â | ||
+ | Medical sciences 10 127.0 116-134 46.22 2.15Â | ||
+ | Physics 20 127.7 112-136 39.05 1.4Â | ||
+ | Social sciences 10 121.8 112-132 41.55 2.04Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | "The scores range from 110 to 141 with a mean of 126.5 and a standard deviation of 6.3 points. All the scores fall within three standard deviations of the mean. The distribution of the scores in terms of Wechsler's classification show that all the scientists obtained scores above the seventieth percentile rank for the general population -- 35.2 per cent are classified as "very superior", 51.3 per cent as "superior", and 13.5 per cent as "bright normal". Approximate percentages of these three groups in a general population sample are 2.2, 6.7, and 16.1 respectively."Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The following chart is taken from //Wechsler's Measurement and Appraisal of | ||
+ | Adult Intelligence// by Joseph D. Matarazzo, 5th edition, p 177. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | XXÂ | ||
+ | X XX X XÂ | ||
+ | X XX X X XÂ | ||
+ | X X XX X X X XÂ | ||
+ | XXXXXXX X XXXXX XÂ | ||
+ | XXXXXXXX XXXXXXXXX XXÂ | ||
+ | X XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX X X Â | ||
+ | | | | | | |Â | ||
+ | 100 110 120 130 140 150Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The Full Scale WAIS IQs for 80 medical students. Adapted from Kole, D. M. | ||
+ | A study of intellectual and personality characteristics of medical students. | ||
+ | (//The Journal of Medical Education//, 1965, 40, 1130-1143)Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | "...for individuals in a profession known to require high intellectual prowess there is, nevertheless, a wide variability around the median IQ of 125.5. The actual range of Full Scale IQ on the WAIS for these young physicians is from a low of 111 (77th percentile) to a high of 149 (99.9th percentile). | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Matarazzo goes on to say, "Another point which is well understood among the few currently growing group of specialists in this country who deal primarily with the assessment of individuals applying for entry into medicine, law, graduate nursing, physics, economics, psychology, English literature, graduate business school, and a host of related scientific and humanistic disciplines is that, on the basis of a purely intellectual index, such applicants are so similar as to have come from the same population. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Currently practicing physicians are also similar to these young medical students in Full Scale WAIS IQs. (Matarazzo and Goldstein, //Journal of Medical Education//, 1972, 47, 102-111)Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Holt and Luborsky in //Personality Patterns of Psychiatrists//, 1958, p. 40, reported that the "Average Wechsler-Bellevue Scale of Adult Intelligence was 128" ... "Scores ranged from 110 to 145. IQ was not decisive in gaining admittance; the average of those accepted was only slightly higher than those who were rejected. The average verbal subscore was 131 with a range of 115-145."Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Balinsky and Shaw (//Personnel Psychology//, 1956, 9, 207-209) reported on an appraisal of top level executives a Full Scale IQ of 124.09, S.D. 7.90; Verbal Scale IQ 125.1, S.D. 8.09; and Performance IQ 117.09, S.D. 9.81. They also reported that only the Verbal IQ was significantly correlated with performance ratings (r = .32, p<.05). However, the subscale of arithmetical reasoning proved to be the best predictor of management performance (r = .42, p<.01). | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Up to this point I have tried to avoid using data from less demanding vocations. But because the article by L.M. Simon and E.A. Levitt (//Occupations//, 1950, 29, 23-25) is the most extensive set of norms I've ever seen in one place for the W-B, I've decided to include their full table. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | SCORES ON THE WECHSLER BELLEVUE SCALE IN RELATION TO OCCUPATION Â | ||
+ | Group N Range 10% Q-1 Median Q-3 90% Â | ||
+ | Engineers 52Â | ||
+ | Full 116-148 121 127 133 135 140Â | ||
+ | Verbal 110-144 120 125 130 136 140Â | ||
+ | Performance 114-145 119 124 129 133 137Â | ||
+ | Professionals I 52Â | ||
+ | Full 113-141 120 126 132 134 137Â | ||
+ | Verbal 109-144 121 126 134 136 141Â | ||
+ | Performance 99-137 115 119 124 129 132Â | ||
+ | Educators 45Â | ||
+ | Full 104-141 118 123 129 134 137Â | ||
+ | Verbal 106-143 112 123 129 134 137Â | ||
+ | Performance 95-139 112 117 125 132 135Â | ||
+ | Professionals II 61Â | ||
+ | Full 106-143 117 123 128 133 138Â | ||
+ | Verbal 112-143 117 123 128 132 137Â | ||
+ | Performance 94-141 111 118 124 129 136Â | ||
+ | Teachers 421Â | ||
+ | Full 94-152 114 120 126 132 137Â | ||
+ | Verbal 94-145 113 120 126 131 137Â | ||
+ | Performance 83-161 108 115 123 130 134Â | ||
+ | Social Service 66Â | ||
+ | Full 108-145 117 121 125 132 135Â | ||
+ | Verbal 105-145 114 118 124 133 136Â | ||
+ | Performance 95-146 106 113 122 129 134Â | ||
+ | Managers 134Â | ||
+ | Full 92-146 113 120 125 130 136Â | ||
+ | Verbal 81-140 110 118 124 131 135Â | ||
+ | Performance 91-154 109 115 122 129 135Â | ||
+ | Nurses 191Â | ||
+ | Full 93-141 113 118 124 128 132Â | ||
+ | Verbal 99-143 110 115 121 127 131Â | ||
+ | Performance 78-145 102 114 125 130 136Â | ||
+ | Arts 62Â | ||
+ | Full 74-147 109 117 124 131 138Â | ||
+ | Verbal 73-142 107 115 120 127 136Â | ||
+ | Performance 78-145 102 114 125 130 136Â | ||
+ | Sales 153Â | ||
+ | Full 95-142 105 112 122 128 132Â | ||
+ | Verbal 93-143 101 111 120 128 133Â | ||
+ | Performance 93-144 103 111 118 125 130Â | ||
+ | Secretaries 107Â | ||
+ | Full 92-135 108 114 121 125 130Â | ||
+ | Verbal 94-134 107 114 120 125 130Â | ||
+ | Performance 88-136 102 108 118 126 129Â | ||
+ | Bookkeepers 55Â | ||
+ | Full 99-137 105 109 117 125 129Â | ||
+ | Verbal 98-141 102 107 115 125 129Â | ||
+ | Performance 94-141 101 107 114 120 125Â | ||
+ | Clerks 128Â | ||
+ | Full 74-140 97 105 116 122 131Â | ||
+ | Verbal 76-145 99 106 117 122 127Â | ||
+ | Performance 69-140 94 103 114 121 129Â | ||
+ | Office Workers 62Â | ||
+ | Full 88-139 99 108 116 123 128Â | ||
+ | Verbal 89-135 98 104 113 122 127Â | ||
+ | Performance 81-141 97 106 115 124 128Â | ||
+ | Skilled Labor 107Â | ||
+ | Full 87-139 103 110 115 123 127Â | ||
+ | Verbal 82-136 98 106 114 120 127Â | ||
+ | Performance 93-136 102 108 117 122 126Â | ||
+ | Personal Service 57Â | ||
+ | Full 54-130 79 92 106 113 122Â | ||
+ | Verbal 59-132 80 90 104 115 121Â | ||
+ | Performance 54-129 84 99 105 114 119 Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Professionals I -- physicians, dentists, lawyers | ||
+ | Educators -- college deans and instructors, high school and grammar school principals | ||
+ | Professionals II -- pharmacists, accountants | ||
+ | Social Service -- social workers, clergy | ||
+ | Managers -- small business owners, retail store managers, office managers, foremen | ||
+ | business executives, small manufacturers. | ||
+ | Arts -- singers, dancers, musicians, actors, artists, designers, commercial artists | ||
+ | Sales -- all inside and outside sales people, wholesale and retail | ||
+ | Office Workers -- stenographers, comptometer operators, typists, receptionists, | ||
+ | telephone switchboard operators | ||
+ | Skilled Labor -- machinists, automobile and aircraft mechanics, radio and television | ||
+ | repairmen, plumbers, electricians | ||
+ | Personal Service -- barbers, beauticians, waiters, food handlers, soda fountain | ||
+ | attendants, domestics | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | I included this particular reference because there were enough high level professions included in the table to make it of interest to my topic. Readers who are interested in similar scores for a very much larger range of occupations are referred to the Army General Classification Test scores from World War II. most of that data is not based on intellectually demanding occupations and is not included in this paper for that reason. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Just after World War II, Dr. G.M. Gilbert, a psychologist-psychiatrist, was given access to the prisoners who were to be put on trial for war crimes at Nuremberg. Included in his examination was a German version of the American Wechsler-Bellevue Adult Intelligence Test. The results of those tests, presented in the table below, may be found in Dr. Gilbert's book, //Nuremberg Diary//, and also in //The Reich Manual//, p. 363, by Leonard Mosley. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Name IQÂ | ||
+ | 1. Hjalmar Schacht 143Â | ||
+ | 2. Arthur Seyss-Inquart 141Â | ||
+ | 3. Hermann Goering 138Â | ||
+ | 4. Karl Doenitz 138Â | ||
+ | 5. Franz von Papen 134Â | ||
+ | 6. Erich Raeder 134Â | ||
+ | 7. Dr. Hans Frank 130Â | ||
+ | 8. Hans Fritsche 130Â | ||
+ | 9. Baldur von Schirach 130Â | ||
+ | 10. Joachim von Ribbentrop 129Â | ||
+ | 11. Wilhelm Keitel 129Â | ||
+ | 12. Albert Speer 128Â | ||
+ | 13. Alfred Jodl 127Â | ||
+ | 14. Alfred Rosenberg 127Â | ||
+ | 15. Constantin von Neurath 125Â | ||
+ | 16. Walther Funk 124Â | ||
+ | 17. Wilhelm Frick 124Â | ||
+ | 18. Rudolf Hess 120Â | ||
+ | 19. Fritz Sauckel 118Â | ||
+ | 20. Ernst Kaltenbrunner 113Â | ||
+ | 21. Julius Streicher 106Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | It's interesting to speculate on what Adolf Hitler's IQ might have been. I think we're on pretty safe ground in saying that he must have been at least as bright as the top members on this list. If he hadn't been, these wolves would surely have torn him apart. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | TNS accepts a WAIS score of 148 for admission, but does not accept Wechsler-Bellevue scores (//Vidya// #7, Nov 1979). However, if TNS did accept such scores, the admission requirement would be about 143. The W-B has a standard deviation of about 14 points. This places the median score (129) of the Nazi leaders just above the 98th percentile, and the highest scoring of the group just below TNS admission requirements. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | It's also interesting to not that Dr. Gilbert was one of Terman's research subjects. He can be easily identified from the clues offered on page 365 of //The Gifted Child Grows Up// (5th edition, 1976). | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The evidence presented so far supports two conclusions. First, that practical success in even the most intellectually demanding professions, such as scientist, professor, physician, or high level executive requires an intelligence quotient no higher than the 96th percentile (125 WAIS). Second, that even in these demanding professions, there is a broad range of ability. Some doctorates were even awarded to individuals with IQs below 100, although this was rare. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | It may have occurred to the reader that the surprisingly low showings may be the result of insufficient ceiling on the tests cited. Unfortunately, this is not the case. Neither the WAIS nor the W-B were designed to measure into the stratosphere of human ability, it's true, but the means and medians cited were all more than a standard deviation below test ceilings. The AGCT has an even higher ceiling than the WAIS or the W-B. Consequently, the average IQ of 125 can't be interpreted as an artifact of the measurement process. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | It may also have occurred to the reader that a moderately low minimum IQ for the professions still doesn't imply that a 160 IQ isn't a better indicator of potential success than a 125 IQ. Unfortunately, the evidence indicates that, while IQ does count for something above 125, it doesn't count for much in terms of practical success. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The strongest and most easily understood evidence for this conclusion comes from the Terman study itself. Terman divided 750 of his subjects into three groups which he called A, B, and C. The A subjects were the most successful members of his group, and the C subjects were his least successful. These evaluations were made by three judges whose primary criterion was the extent to which a subject made use of his superior intellectual ability. Professional recognition counted heavily, but earned income was discounted even in evaluating businessmen. There were 150 subjects in the A and C groups, and the remainder were in the B group. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The A group included all those subjects listed in Who's Who or American Men of Science, teachers above the rank of university instructor, those outstandingly successful in law, medicine, engineering, business, and some of those successful in literature, art, and motion pictures. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The C group was not composed exclusively of failures by any means. In fact, by ordinary standards most of these men could not be considered failures at all. Nevertheless, they were subjects that had failed to live up to their potential. Some were classed as professionals or semi-professionals, but a large number were in skilled trades, clerical and minor business positions, and some were policemen or firemen. Although there were more of the mentally and socially maladjusted among the Cs, there were also many who were well adjusted and contented. Often the difference between a C and an A was nothing more than level of aspiration. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The most important fact for our purpose is that the childhood IQs of these two groups differed by only five points. The adult differences in Concept Mastery Test score (form A) was 18.3 raw score points. Both differences are highly significant statistically, but are too small in either case to be of any practical importance. In short, there was no important difference in intelligence between Terman's most successful subjects and his least successful ones. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The ineffectiveness of IQs above 140 can also be corroborated by a host of correlation studies. Unfortunately, this data is not easily tabulated, and a summary of the results depends upon the reader's understanding of the statistical phenomena of range restriction. Rather than present a tedious mathematical analysis, it seems better to me to sketch an easily understood analogy, and then to draw the obvious parallels. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Suppose that you wished to predict how well potential basketball players would perform. Among the things that you would look for would be players' physical heights. One could say with near certainty that a player who was seven feet tall would perform better than a potential player who was only five feet tall. We would expect the correlation between players' heights and measures of player's effectiveness to be quite high, perhaps .8 or .9. This correlation is, of course, computed from the full range of potential players' heights. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Now, suppose we selected players from only the very tallest candidates. Suppose we selected only those that were at least six foot six. We would find that the seven foot player's performance, when compared with a player who was only six foot six, would not necessarily be superior. Other factors now begin to have overriding importance: who is better motivated, faster, more accurate, etc. The six foot six player could easily turn out to be the better player. Moreover, the correlation between height and player effectiveness has now shrunk to, say, .3 or .4. This shrinkage of the correlation coefficient is the mathematical consequence of shrinkage in range; this is the phenomenon of range restriction. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | In a similar way, when only those at the very top of the IQ spectrum are selected out for some intellectually demanding task, the predictive power of the test will be less. If two young men both graduate from medical school at the same time, one with an IQ of 125 and an undistinguished academic record, and the other with an IQ of 160 and a brilliant record, we would have no way of knowing who would be the more successful physician ten years later. Other factors would have assumed overriding importance: things like good bedside manner, good health, connections, specialty, ambition, etc. The more gifted physician would have an edge in the comparison, but it would be a very slight edge. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | In almost every investigation of IQ with real world performance, two conclusions almost always stand out. First, the correlation of IQ with training criteria (grades, credentials, teacher evaluations, etc.) is almost always higher than with performance on the job. This is another example of range restriction. Training often depends heavily on what IQ tests measure, but performance on the job also depends on personality traits, and special abilities, with IQ becoming less important. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The second finding is that IQ does correlate with performance on the job; correlations being near zero in menial jobs, and about .3 to .5 in the most intellectually demanding jobs. But even here, the strength of the correlation coefficients aren't very impressive, and other, more personal traits can be expected to be of more practical importance than measured intelligence. In fact, E.E. Ghiselli says, "The correlation between IQ and job success in a given occupation is only about .20; this should be compared with the correlation of .50 typically found between IQ and occupational attainment -- taking into account different occupations."Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | In short, after job training or formal education, IQs become relatively ineffective predictors of success. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | For the reader who is interested in the subject of predicting job performance by means of IQ and other kinds of tests, I highly recommend E.E. Ghiselli's book, //The Validity of Occupational Aptitude Tests//, New York; Wiley, 1966. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | The strongest, clearest, and most comprehensive summary of all these research finding can probably be found in A.R. Jensen's book, //Bias in Mental Testing//, 1980, p. 113. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | >"Although IQs are an interval scale, the practical, social, economic and career implications of different IQs most certainly do not represent equal intervals. Again, this is not a fault of the IQ scale, but is the result of personal and societal values and demands. The implications and consequences of, say, a 30-point IQ difference is more significant between IQs of 70 and 100 than between IQs of 130 and 160. The importance of a given difference depends not only on its magnitude, but on whether or not it crosses over any of the social, educational, and occupational thresholds of IQ. To be sure, these thresholds are statistical and represent only differing probabilities for individuals' falling on either side of the threshold. But the differential probabilities are not negligible. Such probabilistic thresholds of this type occur in different regions of the IQ scale, not by arbitrary convention or definition, but because of the structure of the educational and occupational systems of modern industrial societies and their correlated demands on the kind of cognitive ability measured by IQ tests. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | >The four socially and personally most important most important threshold regions on the IQ scale are those that differentiate with high probability between persons who, because of their level of general mental ability, can or cannot attend a regular school (about IQ 50), can or cannot master the traditional subject matter of elementary school (about IQ 75), can or cannot succeed in the academic or college preparatory curriculum through high school (about IQ 105), can or cannot graduate from an accredited four-year college with grades that would qualify for admission to a professional or graduate school (about IQ 115). beyond this, the IQ level becomes relatively unimportant in terms of ordinary occupational aspirations and criteria of success. That is not to say that there are not real differences between the intellectual capabilities represented by IQs of 115 and 150 or even between IQs of 150 and 180. But IQ differences in this upper part of the scale have far less personal implications than the thresholds just described and are generally of lesser importance for success in the popular sense than are certain traits of personality and character. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | >The social implications of exceptionally high ability and its interaction with the other factors that make for unusual achievements are considerably greater than the personal implications. The quality of a society's culture is highly determined by the very small fraction of its population that is most exceptionally endowed. The growth of civilization, the development of written language and of mathematics, the great religious and philosophic insights, scientific discoveries, practical inventions, industrial developments, advancements in legal and political systems, and the world's masterpieces of literature, architecture, music and painting, it seems safe to say, are attributable to a rare small proportion of the human population throughout history who undoubtedly possessed, in addition to other important qualities of talent, energy, and imagination, a high level of the essential mental ability measured by tests of intelligence."Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Terman was right; an IQ above 140 has very little significance in terms of personal success. But if Jensen's assessment is correct, and I am confident that it is, where were the historical personages that should have emerged from Terman's study? | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Terman suffered badly from halo effect He not only exaggerated the importance of IQ, he also inflated test scores. All of his subjects were chosen as children of approximately 11 years of age. Their childhood IQs were reported to be 152, but Quinn McNemar reported their adult IQs to be only 134, 18 points less than their childhood scores (//The Gifted Child Grows Up//, p. 146). Yet adult IQs were avoided as often as possible in reporting adult achievements; childhood IQs were preferred in every case. Other investigators making use of Terman's data almost always follow this practice as well. Consequently, a myth has grown up reflecting a superiority that Terman's group did not in fact possess. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Terman also selected members of his group with IQs above 170 (average 177.7 men and 177.6 women) for special study. If we subtract the same 18 point difference from 170 that we found between childhood IQs and adult IQs in the main group, we will arrive at an estimate of 152: an estimate less than the TNS average of 156. But even 152 is an overestimate for two reasons. First, the farther a score is from the mean, the greater the regression to the mean will be: 18 points is an underestimate. Second, Terman says specifically that an IQ of 170 is found about 3 times in 10,000 of the general population (//The Gifted Child Grows Up//, p. 282). That's 3.43 sigma according to my tables, or about 155 IQ, before the 18 or more points are subtracted to allow for adult regression. The average TNS member is at least the equal of Terman's high group, and may very well be their superior. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Nevertheless, Terman's "Subjects of IQ 170 or Above" is the only group that I'm aware of that comes close to furnishing a picture of what TNS members are probably like. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | Jensen's reference to the social implications of exceptionally high IQs suggests that intellectual achievements on an historical scale will be made only by those of very rare ability indeed. Can we find such achievements and measure the IQs of those who made them? Indeed we can. In fact, it's already been done. In the early 1950s, Doctor Anne Roe wrote a book titled //The Making of a Scientist// (Greenwood Press, Westport Conn, 1973) in which she investigated the biographies, personalities, and intellectual abilities of 64 of America's most eminent living scientists, some of whom were Nobel Prize winners. She found the average verbal IQ of this very eminent group to be 166 (p. 164). These scientists comprise the only research group I've come across whose intellectual abilities resemble those of the Four Sigma Society. | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | CODAÂ | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | If IQs above 140 have little importance for personal achievement, what then is left for us? Jensen says, "The evidence is overwhelming that scholastic achievement increases linearly as a function of IQ throughout the entire range of the IQ scale..." (//Bias in Mental Testing//, p. 319). In other words, what's left for us is knowledge. We are the ones who learn, who understand, who discover, and who invent. That should be ample challenge for any of us. </spoiler>Â | ||
+ | * [[http://www.freerepublic.com/focus/f-chat/2242985/posts|Theories of Multiple Intelligence]]Â | ||
+ | Â | ||
+ | <spoiler |Theories of Multiple Intelligence – texte complet >The mathematical technique called factor analysis was invented by psychologists specifically to answer the age old question, "Is there more than one kind of intelligence?" We now know that there are two: one called fluid g, measured by culture fair tests such as the Raven Progressive Matrices or the LAIT, and another called crystallized g, measured by culture loaded tests like the Concept Mastery Test or the Miller Analogies Test. What we call g has been defined as the ability to "educe relations and correlates," or in more everyday terms, the abilities for inductive ("relations") and deductive ("correlates") reasoning. Culture fair tests measure the ability to educe relations and correlates using abstract diagrams, and other material that requires only a minimum of formal learning. Culture loaded tests measure the ability to educe relations and correlates using learned and over learned material, such as vocabulary, algorithms for arithmetic or multiplication, recognition of common objects and their uses, etc. Ordinary IQ tests measure both kinds of intelligence, but not necessarily to the same degree; they are generally biased in favor of crystallized g.\\ | ||
\\ | \\ | ||
The currently accepted relationship between these two kinds of ability is called the investment theory of intelligence. It says, in effect, that we are all born with a certain raw ability, or the eduction of relations and correlates, which can be measured with culture fair tests. As we get older, we "invest" this fluid g in certain kinds of judgment skills, such as those involved in doing a mathematical word problem, or parsing a sentence. When we are young, the theory goes, our formal educations are so much alike that we all invest our fluid g in much the same kinds of judgment skills. That means that our fluid intelligence and our crystallized intelligence are so similar at an early age that it's almost impossible to tell them apart. After we leave school, however, we all begin to invest our fluid g abilities in different things. Measures of fluid g and crystallized g begin to draw apart. Those that invest their fluid g in school-like activities, such as accounting or law, continue to show intellectual growth on conventional (crystallized) IQ tests. Those that put their intelligence to work in other ways, such as becoming ranchers or artists, will not show the same intellectual growth, and may even show a decline in IQ on conventional measures of intelligence. \\ | The currently accepted relationship between these two kinds of ability is called the investment theory of intelligence. It says, in effect, that we are all born with a certain raw ability, or the eduction of relations and correlates, which can be measured with culture fair tests. As we get older, we "invest" this fluid g in certain kinds of judgment skills, such as those involved in doing a mathematical word problem, or parsing a sentence. When we are young, the theory goes, our formal educations are so much alike that we all invest our fluid g in much the same kinds of judgment skills. That means that our fluid intelligence and our crystallized intelligence are so similar at an early age that it's almost impossible to tell them apart. After we leave school, however, we all begin to invest our fluid g abilities in different things. Measures of fluid g and crystallized g begin to draw apart. Those that invest their fluid g in school-like activities, such as accounting or law, continue to show intellectual growth on conventional (crystallized) IQ tests. Those that put their intelligence to work in other ways, such as becoming ranchers or artists, will not show the same intellectual growth, and may even show a decline in IQ on conventional measures of intelligence. \\ | ||
Ligne 347: | Ligne 639: | ||
Source : http://www.prometheussociety.org</spoiler> | Source : http://www.prometheussociety.org</spoiler> | ||
- | * Intelligence and g | + | * [[http://miyaguchi.4sigma.org/gradytowers/iq_intell.html|IQ vs. Intelligence]] |
- | <spoiler |Intelligence and g - texte complet >Did you know that //Homo erectus// had an IQ of about 45? This is not just a guess, but the result of an experiment carried out by Thomas Wynn, an anthropologist at the University of Colorado.\\ | + | <spoiler |IQ vs. Intelligence – texte complet >Did you know that //Homo erectus// had an IQ of about 45? This is not just a guess, but the result of an experiment carried out by Thomas Wynn, an anthropologist at the University of Colorado.\\ |
\\ | \\ | ||
//Homo erectus// produced a category of stone tool that anthropologists call the Acheulean tool industry. Dr. Wynn discovered that it takes a mental age of at least seven years to learn how to reproduce an Acheulean tool. Using the classic formula\\ | //Homo erectus// produced a category of stone tool that anthropologists call the Acheulean tool industry. Dr. Wynn discovered that it takes a mental age of at least seven years to learn how to reproduce an Acheulean tool. Using the classic formula\\ | ||
Ligne 360: | Ligne 652: | ||
IQ tests measure something real and something terribly important, but they do not assess all of what is called intelligence. Many important mental abilities are left out. Abilities responsible for art, music, dance, cooking, mechanical invention, clerical exactness, foreign languages, caring for a baby, defeating an enemy in war, and so on, have little connection with IQ. They have little connection because literacy and numeracy have little to do with excellence in these fields.\\ | IQ tests measure something real and something terribly important, but they do not assess all of what is called intelligence. Many important mental abilities are left out. Abilities responsible for art, music, dance, cooking, mechanical invention, clerical exactness, foreign languages, caring for a baby, defeating an enemy in war, and so on, have little connection with IQ. They have little connection because literacy and numeracy have little to do with excellence in these fields.\\ | ||
\\ | \\ | ||
- | IQ tests are powerful predictors only in the fields in which literacy and numeracy are of central importance. These are the core abilities responsible for the creation, maintenance and progress of civilization. Without them there could be no literature, law, religion, philosophy. There could also be no mathematics, science, technology, market economy, computer science, etc. No one could have more respect for these qualities than I have, but I don't mistake them for an index of human worth. There are other mental qualities of equal worth not assessed by IQ tests.\\Â | + | IQ tests are powerful predictors only in the fields in which literacy and numeracy are of central importance. These are the core abilities responsible for the creation, maintenance and progress of civilization. Without them there could be no literature, law, religion, philosophy. There could also be no mathematics, science, technology, market economy, computer science, etc. No one could have more respect for these qualities than I have, but I don't mistake them for an index of human worth. There are other mental qualities of equal worth not assessed by IQ tests.</spoiler> |
- | \\Â | + | |
- | Source : http://www.eskimo.com/~miyaguch/grady/iq_intell.html</spoiler> | + | |
- | Â | + | * [[https://web.archive.org/web/20130902025647/http://www.eskimo.com/~miyaguch/grady/realworld.html|IQ and real world success]]Â |
- | * [[http://www.eskimo.com/~miyaguch/grady/iq_intell.html|IQ vs. Intelligence]]Â | + | * [[http://miyaguchi.4sigma.org/gradytowers/diff_intell.html|Different Kinds of Intelligence]]Â |
- | * [[http://www.eskimo.com/~miyaguch/grady/realworld.html|IQ and real world success]]Â | + | * [[http://miyaguchi.4sigma.org/gradytowers/societies.html|On using multiple tests for high IQ society admissions]] and [[http://miyaguchi.4sigma.org/gradytowers/followup.html|Followup]]Â |
- | * [[http://www.eskimo.com/~miyaguch/grady/diff_intell.html|Different Kinds of Intelligence]]Â | + | * [[http://miyaguchi.4sigma.org/gradytowers/about_rasch.html|About Rasch scores]]Â |
- | * [[http://www.eskimo.com/~miyaguch/grady/societies.html|On using multiple tests for high IQ society admissions]]Â | + | * [[http://miyaguchi.4sigma.org/gradytowers/howtonorm.html|How To Norm a Super-High IQ Test]] |
- | * [[http://www.eskimo.com/~miyaguch/grady/about_rasch.html|About Rasch scores]] | + | |
* [[http://www.megasociety.net/noesis/149/iq&pear.html|IQ, Creativity and the Twisted Pear, or Why the Sidekick Gets the Girl]] | * [[http://www.megasociety.net/noesis/149/iq&pear.html|IQ, Creativity and the Twisted Pear, or Why the Sidekick Gets the Girl]] | ||
+ | * [[http://prometheussociety.org/cms/index.php/articles/da-capo|Da Capo]] | ||
===== Annexes ===== | ===== Annexes ===== | ||
==== Articles connexes ==== | ==== Articles connexes ==== | ||
+ | |||
[[articles:THQI]] -- Problèmes fréquemment rencontrés, témoignages, positionnement social \\ | [[articles:THQI]] -- Problèmes fréquemment rencontrés, témoignages, positionnement social \\ | ||
[[articles:qi]] | [[articles:qi]] |